1 / 24

Coping with Achievement-Related Failure: An Examination of Conversations Between Friends

Coping with Achievement-Related Failure: An Examination of Conversations Between Friends. Ellen Rydell Altermatt, Elizabeth Broady, & Taryn Bellgard Hanover College Funded by National Science Foundation Grant BCS-0236678. Responses to Achievement-Related Failure.

Download Presentation

Coping with Achievement-Related Failure: An Examination of Conversations Between Friends

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. Coping with Achievement-Related Failure: An Examination of Conversations Between Friends Ellen Rydell Altermatt, Elizabeth Broady, & Taryn Bellgard Hanover College Funded by National Science Foundation Grant BCS-0236678

  2. Responses to Achievement-Related Failure • Mastery-oriented approach (Dweck, 1986) • Maintain high expectations for future success • Report positive affect • Demonstrate persistence in the face of challenge • Learned helpless approach (Dweck, 1986) • Have diminished expectations for future success • Report negative affect • Fail to persist in the face of challenge

  3. What Role Do Social Interactions Play? • Hokoda and Fincham (1995) • Mothers of mastery-oriented children were more likely to offer assistance when their children requested it. • Mothers of mastery-oriented children were less likely to respond to self-critical statements (e.g., “I can’t do it.”) by suggesting that their children discontinue the activity.

  4. Why Friends? • Children spend a substantial amount of time with friends(e.g., Larson & Richards, 1991) • Children seek the support and advice of friends during times of stress (e.g., Causey & Dubow, 1992)

  5. Research Questions • What are the features of children’s conversations with friends following achievement-related failure? • Are the features of children’s conversations related to changes in their responses to failure over time?

  6. Participants • Fourth, fifth, and sixth grade students • 116 friendship dyads • 40 male dyads, 76 female dyads • 70% Caucasian, 14% African-American, 7% Latino

  7. Procedure • Ice-breaker activity • Children worked on puzzles in separate rooms • Focal child received unsolvable puzzles • Friend received either solvable (success condition) or unsolvable puzzles (failure condition) • Children were reunited to discuss the task • Children work on a final set of solvable problems

  8. Questionnaires • Mastery-Orientated Beliefs • Baseline, Post-Failure, Post-discussion • Sample Items • “I want to do the puzzles.” • “I am confident that I will do well on the puzzles.” • Reliability: αs = .81 to .91

  9. Coding Children’s Conversations • Overview • 17,000 statements (κs = .71 to .99) • 8,441 focal child statements • 8,559 friend statements • 75% of statements were on-task

  10. Statement Types • Performance Checks (e.g., How many [puzzles] did you solve?) • Positive Performance Statements (e.g., I got them all.) • Negative Performance Statements (e.g., I didn’t get any of mine.) • Positive Self-Evaluative Statements (e.g. I’m really good at puzzles.) • Negative Self-Evaluative Statements (e.g., I stink at puzzles.) • Discounting Statements (e.g., I’m used to doing puzzles that attach.) • Help Seeking (e.g., How do you make the diamond?) • Help Giving (e.g., Okay. Well, you just need to look for a green one.)

  11. Descriptive Analyses • Mastery-oriented beliefs • Features of conversations

  12. Mastery-Orientated Beliefs

  13. Descriptive Analyses • Mastery-oriented beliefs • Features of conversations

  14. Gender Differences in Performance Checks

  15. Predicting Mastery-Oriented Beliefs • Hierarchical regression analyses • Dependent variable • Mastery-oriented beliefs at post-discussion • Control variable • Mastery-oriented beliefs at post-failure • Predictor variables • Statement type • Gender (male, female) • Condition (friend success, friend failure)

  16. Predicting Mastery-Oriented Beliefs • Statement Type Main Effect • Discounting statements (FC), β= -.15, p < .01 • Help-giving statements (Friend), β= .12, p < .01

  17. Predicting Mastery Orientation • Statement Type x Gender Interactions • Negative performance statements (FC), β= -.28, p < .001 • Negative self-evaluative statements (FC), β= -.19, p < .01

  18. Negative Performance Statements

  19. Negative Self-Evaluative Statements

  20. Why the gender difference? • Sequential analyses • What happens immediately after each statement type? • Are particular sequences of statements more likely to occur with boys than with girls?

  21. Sequential AnalysesNegative Performance Statements

  22. Sequential AnalysesNegative Self-Evaluative Statements

  23. Sample Conversation Between Girls • FC: I can’t put puzzles together. As a matter of fact I think that I may need to practice a little more. • FR: I know… This was hard. • FC: [Laughs] …. I mean, I hated it. I was like, ok do this, do this, and then she was like, ‘I’m sorry, but your time is up.’ • FR: I know, she was like, ‘It’s time for the next one.’ I was like, um. • FC: [Laughs]. Shoot! • FR: And then …. it was time for the next one. • FC: …I’m still shaking from doing it.

  24. Co-RuminationRose (2002) • Co-rumination is characterized by • repeated discussion of the same problem • mutual encouragement of discussing problem • Girls are more likely to co-ruminate than are boys • Co-rumination has tradeoffs

More Related