390 likes | 596 Views
The influence of L1 syntax on L2 processing. L1. L2. Alice Foucart. ESRC Centre for Research on Bilingualism, Bangor University – 7 th March 2011. Outline:. Part 1 Gender processing in L2: review Experiments gender processing in L2 (ERPs and eye-tracking) Part 2
E N D
The influence of L1 syntax on L2 processing L1 L2 Alice Foucart ESRC Centre for Research on Bilingualism, Bangor University – 7th March 2011
Outline: • Part 1 • Gender processing in L2: review • Experiments gender processing in L2 (ERPs and eye-tracking) Part 2 • Agreement processing in L2 production: conceptual gender in possessive structures Conclusions
Grammatical gender English: the car French: lafemvoiturefem Spanish: elmasccochemasc German: dasneuAutoneu
Research questions: • Is gender represented and processed in a similar manner in native and non-native speakers? • Is gender processing in L2 affected by the L1?
Gender processing in L1 Example: Lafem / *lemasc cleffem était dans la serrure The key was in the keyhole P600 effect (syntactic integration) (Barber & Carreiras, 2005; Foucart & Frenck-Mestre, 2004; Gunter et al., 2000; Hagoort & Brown, 1999) Sometimes preceded by a LAN (morpho-syntactic violations) (Barber & Carreiras, 2005; Gunter et al., 2000) = Gender is syntactically processed
Comprehension in L2: ERPs studies Difficulties in lexical-semantic integration • Similar but with reduced amplitude and later peak latency • (Ardal et al. 1990; Hahne & Friederici, 2001; Weber-Fox & Neville, 1996) • L2 learning (Osterhout et al., 2006) Word category violations • Sometimes found, and often with different topography • (Hahne, Müller & Clahsen, 2006; Rossi et al., 2006; Weber-Fox & Neville, 1996) Morpho-syntactic violations Syntactic integration • Found in some studies, but not in other • (Foucart & Frenck-Mestre, 2004; Hahne, 2001, but Hahne & Friederici, 2001; • Weber-Fox & Neville, 1996). Semantic processing: similar ≠ Syntactic processing: Proficiency and AoA
Syntactic processing in L2 - Different L1 and L2 processing Shallow structure hypothesis (Clahsen & Felser, 2006) The syntactic analysis processed by late L2 speakers during language comprehension is not as ‘full’ as that processed by native speakers. • Similar L1 and L2 processing • Three-phases model (Friederici, 2002; Rossi, Gugler, Friederici, Hahne, 2006 ) • With enough exposure, highly proficient L2 learners can reach native-like processing levels even if they learned their L2 late in life.
Sabourin & Stowe’s (2008) study Participants: Dutch native speakers, German-Dutch and Romance-Dutch learners Materials: Determiner-noun gender agreement violation e.g. Hetneu/*Decom kleine kindcomprobeerde voor het eerst te lopen. The small child tried to walk for the first time Results: Dutch native speakers and German-Dutch learners → P600 effect Romance-Dutch learners → No effect Conclusions: Automatic gender processing in L2 not only depends on the presence of a grammatical gender system in the L1 but also requires overlapping of lexical gender. Sabourin, L., & Stowe, L A. (2008) Second language processing: When are L1 and L2 processed similarly. Second Language Research, 24 (3), 397-430.
Foucart & Frenck-Mestre’s (2011) study Participants: French native speakers and German-French advanced learners Materials: Determiner-noun gender agreement violation e.g., Hier lafem/*lemascchaisefem était dans le salon; Yesterday the chair was in the living room Results: French native speakers and German-French advanced learners → P600 effect Conclusions: With enough exposure, proficient later learners can process gender in a native-like manner. Foucart, A. & Frenck-Mestre, C. (2011). Grammatical gender processing in L2: Electrophysiological evidence of the effect of L1 - L2 syntactic similarity. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, DOI:10.1017/S136672891000012X
Tokowicz & MacWhinney’s (2005)study Participants: English-Spanish learners Materials: (a) tense-marking (similar in English and Spanish) e.g., Su abuela *cocinando/cocina muy bien His grandmother *cooking/cooks very well. (b) nominal number agreement (different in English and Spanish) e.g., *El/Los niños están jugando The boys are playing. (c) nominal gender agreement (unique to Spanish) e.g., Ellos fueron a *unmasc/unafem fiestafem They went to a party. → P600 effect → No effect → P600 effect Conclusions: - Online sensitivity to gender agreement in L2 learners despite the absence of grammatical gender in their L1. - Features that are not present in L1 should be acquired faster than those that are in conflict with L2 features. Tokowicz, N., & MacWhinney, B. (2005). Implicit and explicit measures of sensitivity to violations in second language grammar: An event related potential investigation. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 2, 173–204.
Gillon Dowens, Vergara, Barber, Carreiras’ (2009)study Participants: Spanish native speakers, English-Spanish late learners (+ 12 yrs exposure) Materials: Determiner-noun gender agreement violation e.g., Elmasc /*lafem /*losplur suelomasc-sing está plano y bien acabado, The floor is flat and well-finished Noun-predicative adjective gender agreement violation e.g., El suelo masc-singestá planomasc/*planafem/*planosplur y bien acabado Results: Determiner-noun gender agreement violation Similar pattern for native and non-native speakers → LAN + P600 effects Noun-predicative adjective gender agreement violation Native speakers → LAN + P600 effects; L2 speakers → P600 effect • Conclusions: • With enough exposure, late L2 learners can reach native-like processing even when agreement involves features that do not exist in the learner’s L1. • Processing non-local agreement may be more demanding in L2 (working memory). Gillon-Dowens, M., Vergara, M., Barber, H. A., Carreiras, M. (2010).Morpho-syntactic processing in late L2 learners. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22 (8), 1870–1887
Keating’s (2009)study Participants: Spanish native speakers, English-Spanish beginner, intermediate and advanced learners. • Materials: • in the determiner phrase • Una casafem pequeñafem/* pequeñomasc [cuesta mucho en San Francisco.], • A small house costs a lot in San Francisco. • (b) in the verbal phrase • La casafem [es bastante pequeñafem /*pequeñomasc y necesita muchas reparaciones.] • The house is quite small and needs a lot of repairs. • (c) in a subordinate clause • Una casafem [VP cuesta menos [CP si [VP es pequeñafem/*pequeñomasc y necesita reparaciones]]. • A house costs less if it is small and needs repairs. Keating, G. D. (2009). Sensitivity to violations of gender agreement in native and nonnative Spanish: An eye-movement investigation. Language Learning, 59, 503-535.
Keating’s (2009)study Results: (a) in the determiner phrase Beginner and Intermediate learners: → no effect. Native speakers and advanced learners → longer fixation times for violations (b) in the verbal phrase L2 learners: → no effect. Native speakers → longer fixation times for violations (c) in a subordinate clause L2 learners: → no effect. Native speakers → longer fixation times for violations • Conclusions: • High proficient late L2 learners can acquire new abstract grammatical features such as gender in their L2. • Processing non-local agreement may be more demanding in L2 because of the working memory cost. Keating, G. D. (2009). Sensitivity to violations of gender agreement in native and nonnative Spanish: An eye-movement investigation. Language Learning, 59, 503-535.
L2 gender processing: Evidence from ERPs and eye-tracking studies - Gender processing in L2 can only reach native-like level if the syntactic structures are similar in L1 and L2. (Sabourin & Haverkort, 2003; Sabourin & Stowe, 2008) - Features that are not present in L1 (e.g., gender for native English speakers) should be acquired faster than those that are in conflict with L2 features. (Gillon Dowens et al. 2010; Tokowicz & MacWhinney, 2005) • With enough exposure, late L2 learners can reach native-like processing even when agreement involves features that do not exist in the learner’s L1. • (Foucart & Frenck-Mestre, 2011; Gillon Dowens et al., 2009; Keating, 2009; Tokowicz & MacWhinney, 2005) • L2 learners may not be able to process gender as ‘fully’ as native speakers in non-local contexts (e.g., predicative adjectives) • (Gillon Dowens et al., 2010; Keating, 2009)
Experiment 1: Method Foucart, A. & Frenck-Mestre, C. (under revision), JML. • Participants (N=14): • French native speakers • English-French advanced learners • German-French advanced learners • Materials: • 96 nouns, inanimate • Same/different gender (French, lacleffem; German, der Schlüsselmasc; the key) • Electrodes (21 scalp sites): • Central, Fz, Cz, Pz • - Frontal, central and posterior areas • of the left hemisphere, • Fp1, F3, F7, Fc5, C3, Cp5, T5, P3, O1 • - of the right hemisphere, • Fp2, F4, F8, Fc6, C4, Cp6, T6, P4, O2
Experiment 1: Conditions Conditions: Correct En ce moment les pommesfem vertesfem sont de saison. IncorrectEn ce moment les pommesfem vertsmasc * sont de saison. At the moment green apples are in season Predictions: + ? • For French native speakers and the two L2 groups
Experiment 1: Results French native speakers English-French bilinguals German-French bilinguals French native speakers: P600 effect Gender syntactically processed English-French bilinguals: P600 effect Gender syntactically processed German-French bilinguals: No effect ? New syntactic structure ? Plural agreement rule in L1
Experiment 2: Method and Predictions Participants (N=14): Same as Experiment 1 Materials and procedure: Same as Experiment 1 Conditions Correct Souvent les / petitesfem/ pommesfem/ sont/ bien/ sucrées. IncorrectSouvent les / petitsmasc*/ pommesfem/ sont/ bien/ sucrées. Usually small apples are very sweet Predictions: • For French native speakers and English-French bilinguals • ? German-bilinguals
Experiment 2: Results French native speakers English-French bilinguals German-French bilinguals French native speakers: P600 effect Gender syntactically processed English-French bilinguals: N400 effect Still acquiring gender agreement with pre-posed adjective German-French bilinguals: No effect X New syntactic structure Plural agreement in L1
Experiments 3: Method and Predictions Participants (N=14): Same as Experiments 1 and 2 Materials and procedure: Same as Experiments 1 and 2 Conditions Correct En automne les /pommesfem/ sont/ vertesfem/ sur cet/ arbre. IncorrectEn automne les /pommesfem/ sont/ vertsmasc*/ sur cet/ arbre. In autumn apples are green on this tree. Predictions: • For French native speakers and English-French bilinguals • BUT no effect for German-bilinguals
Experiment 3: Results French native speakers English-French bilinguals German-French bilinguals French native speakers: P600 effect Gender syntactically processed English-French bilinguals: No effect ? Gender agreement more complex when not in a local context German-French bilinguals: No effect
Experiments 4: Method and Predictions Participants (N=14): Same as Experiments 3 Materials and procedure: Same as Experiments 3 Conditions R1 R2 R3 R4 R5 R6 Correct En automne les /pommesfem/ sont/ vertesfem/ sur cet/ arbre. IncorrectEn automne les /pommesfem/ sont/ vertsmasc*/ sur cet/ arbre. In autumn apples are green on this tree. Predictions: • Longer fixation times for French native speakers • ? English-French bilinguals • BUT no effect for German-bilinguals
Experiment 4: Results R1 R2 R3 R4 R5 R6 En automne les /pommesfem/ sont/ vertesfem-vertsmasc*/ sur cet/ arbre. In autumn apples are green on this tree. • French native speakers: Longer reading times for incorrect agreement • English-French bilinguals: Same pattern as the native speakers • German-French bilinguals: No effect
Experiments 1-4: Conclusions Is gender represented and processed in a similar manner in native and non-native speakers? • Late bilinguals can attain native-like processing (P600, reduced amplitude) • HOWEVER: • Proficiency (N400, complex syntactic structure) • Non local processing seems to be more difficult (working memory). Is gender processing in L2 affected by the L1? • Influence of the native language (gender vs. absence of gender)
Furtherresearch… in production Is processing of conceptual gender agreement as difficult as processing of grammatical gender agreement in L2 ? • Does L1 syntax affects the processing of L2-English gender agreement? • Possessives adjectives/pronouns processing in L2 English
Possessives adjective agreement rules Possessive adjective agreement Agree with the possessor(waitress): English: The waitress chases herFem / *hisMascson Greek: I servitora kiniga ton gio tisFem / *touMasc Agree with thepossessee(son) Spanish: La camarera persigue a suMasc / *suFemhijo French: La serveuse poursuit sonMasc / *saFemfils
Experiment 5: Method (Santesteban, Foucart, Pickering, Branigan, 2010) Participants: 12 English native speakers 24 Spanish-English bilinguals (AoA: 7.4; proficiency: 5.6 (self-rate: 1-low, 7-high)) 24 French-English bilinguals (AoA: 10.6; proficiency: 5.5) 24 Greek-English bilinguals (AoA: 9.1; proficiency: 5.9) Materials: 32 experimental pictures (8 per condition) + 72 fillers Possessor characters: Masculine (e.g., boxer) vs. Feminine (e.g., waitress) Possessee characters: Masculine (e.g., son) vs. Feminine (e.g., daughter) Conditions: 2 (Possessor: masc vs. fem) x 2 (Possessee : masc vs. fem) Gender match The boxer chases his son (masc-masc) The waitress chases her daughter (fem-fem) Gender mismatch The boxer chases his daughter (masc-fem) The waitress chases her son (fem-masc)
Agreement errors Similarly to number (Bock & Miller, 1991), the gender of a local noun can interfere with agreement process and result in gender agreement errors called ‘attraction errors’ (e.g., noun-adjective, Vigliocco & Franck, 1999; Franck et al., 2008; or antecedent-pronoun, Meyer & Bock, 1999) Number: e.g., The picture of the postcards *are/is nice. Gender: e.g., El relojMasc de la ciudadFem es *viejaFem,/ viejomasc The clock of the town is old
Hypotheses Do L1 gender agreement rules affect conceptual gender agreement production of L2 possessive pronouns? • If L2 processing is affected by the ‘weaker’ L2 syntactic representations of bilinguals, we expect no differences between bilingual groups. • If L1 agreement rules affect L2 processing, we expect larger gender attraction effects for Spanish- and French-English bilinguals than for Greek-English bilinguals.
Experiment 5: Method (Santesteban, Foucart, Pickering, Branigan, 2010)
Summary of attraction effects Experiment 5: Results (Santesteban, Foucart, Pickering, Branigan, 2010) Errors • English monolinguals made no errors. • Only bilinguals showed gender attraction effects • Interestingly, similar attraction effects produced by all groups of bilinguals (no significant mismatch x group interaction)
Experiment 5: Conclusions (Santesteban, Foucart, Pickering, Branigan, 2010) L2 gender agreement errors during productionofpossessive adjective seems to be due to ‘weaker’ syntactic representations, not effects of L1 syntax HOWEVER, there was no linear intervention of the possessee attractor.
Possessives pronoun agreement rules Possessive pronoun agreement Agree with the possessor(waitress): English: The waitress says that the son is hersFem / *hisMasc Greek: I servitora lei oti o gios ine dikos tisFem / *touMasc Agree with thepossessee(son) Spanish: La camareradice que el hijo es suyoMasc/ *suyaFem French: La serveuse dit que le fils est le sienMasc / *sienneFem
Experiment 6: Method (Santesteban, Foucart, Pickering, Branigan, 2010) Participants: 21 English native speakers 21 Spanish-English bilinguals (AoA: 9.8; English proficiency: 5.5) 21 French-English bilinguals (AoA: 11.5; English proficiency: 5.3) 21 Greek-English bilinguals (AoA: 7.6; English proficiency: 5.8) Materials, Conditions and Procedure: Similar as Experiment 1 Gender match The boxer says the son is his (masc-masc) The waitress says the daughter is hers (fem-fem) Gender mismatch The boxer says the daughter is his (masc-fem) The waitress says the son is hers (fem-masc)
Experiment 6: Method (Santesteban, Foucart, Pickering, Branigan, 2010)
Summary of attraction effects Experiment 6: Results and Conclusions (Santesteban, Foucart, Pickering, Branigan, 2010) Errors • All groups showed gender attraction effects when the possessee attractor linearly intervened between the antecedent and the pronoun. • Importantly, Spanish/French-English bilinguals produced more errors, and showed larger gender attraction effects than Greek-English bilinguals and English monolinguals. • Similar attraction effects for Greek-English bilinguals and English monolinguals. • Production of L2 possessive pronouns is affected by L1 syntax.
Experiments 5 & 6: Conclusions (Santesteban, Foucart, Pickering, Branigan, 2010) Processing of possessive structures is affected by the gender of the possessee- Native speakers: Gender attraction errors only occur with linear intervention of the possessee (however, this might only be a sentence complexity effect)- Bilingual speakers: Gender attraction errors occur independently of the position of the possessee in the sentence. • At least part of bilinguals’ gender agreement error production is due to ‘weaker’ syntactic representations. • However, L1 agreement rules that differ from L2 rules do also affect gender agreement processing in L2.
General conclusions • Is L2 processing influenced by L1 syntax? YES! (Foucart & Frenck-Mestre, 2011; Sabourin, 2003; Santesteban et al., 2010) HOWEVER: - Proficiency (Foucart, 2008; Osterhout et al., 2006; Santesteban et al., 2010) - Complexity of the structure (Gillon Dowens et al., 2010; Hahne, 2001; Keating, 2009)
THANK YOU! English: the end French: lafemfinfem Spanish: elmascfinmasc German: dasneuEndeneu