340 likes | 697 Views
Sleep and Arousal. Lecture 9 NRS201S John Yeomans. EEG Changes in Sleep. Waking: Alpha (10 Hz) and beta/gamma waves (40 Hz). Slow-Wave sleep: From alpha to spindles (14 Hz) and delta (1-4 Hz). REM sleep: Cortical arousal and muscular atonia. Also called paradoxical or dream sleep.
E N D
Sleep and Arousal Lecture 9 NRS201S John Yeomans
EEG Changes in Sleep • Waking: Alpha (10 Hz) and beta/gamma waves (40 Hz). • Slow-Wave sleep: From alpha to spindles (14 Hz) and delta (1-4 Hz). • REM sleep: Cortical arousal and muscular atonia. Also called paradoxical or dream sleep. • Triggered in pontine reticular formation.
Gamma Alpha Hours
REM Sleep • Brain is active, and eyes are active. • Muscles of body are profoundly inhibited (atonia). • Subjects report dreams, when awoken. • NE and 5HT neurons silent. Ch neurons active. • In slow-wave sleep, brain and eyes are quiet, but muscles are more active.
Brain Areas--Early Studies • Coma (prolonged unconsciousness) due to injury in dorsal reticular formation. • Stimulation of RF leads to arousal. • Ascending path for cortical arousal. • Descending path for atonia. • Critical area in dorsal pontine reticular formation.
Diffuse Arousal Systems • Locus coeruleus Norepinephrine neurons (A6). • Mesopontine Cholinergic neurons (Ch5,6). • Raphe Serotonin neurons (B5,6). • Tuberomammilary Histamine neurons. • Lateral hypothalamus Orexin/Hypocretin neurons. • Basal forebrain Cholinergic neurons (Ch1-4).
Norepinephrine and Serotonin Active in Waking and in Slow-Wave Sleep
Mesopontine Ch5,6 Basal Forebrain Ch1-4) Cholinergic Arousal Systems Active in Waking and REM Sleep
Sleep Disorders • Insomnia (too little sleep). • Sleep apnea (loss of breathing in REM, too much atonia?). • Narcolepsy/cataplexy (daytime sleepiness and REM/atonia attacks). • Triggered by arousal (e.g. laughing, running). • Due to loss of orexin/hypocretin neurons in humans, or receptors in dogs and mice.
Narcolepsy • Orexin 2 receptors lost in dogs (Mignon). • O/H neurons lost in humans. • O/H gene or receptors in mice. • O/H neurons active in waking arousal, and needed to inhibit atonia. • In narcolepsy, arousal can activate REM/atonia neurons, if O/H signal is lost. • Which neurons and how? Ch5,6?
Loss of O/H neurons: Daytime sleepiness, Cataplexy (atonia) induced by arousal.
Circadian Rhythms March 17, 2006 PSY391S John Yeomans
Timing of Motivated Behaviors • When is best season to feed and mate? Seasonal periods of activity and breeding based on availability of food. Based on axis of earth around sun. • When is best time of day to feed? Diurnal/nocturnal to find food and avoid predators. Based on earth’s rotation relative to sun. • Circadian clock built into all plants and animals to help survival.
Rhythms • Endogenous clock: Measured in constant conditions, still 23-25 hr. “free running” • Rhythm is lost when SCN lesioned in mammals, or pineal gland in birds. • Rhythm is restored by transplanting new SCN. Period of donor SCN. • Tau mutant hamster has 20 hr rhythm. • Therefore, SCN is endogenous clock for activity.
Free running 24.1 hr No rhythm Tau mutant SCN 19.8 hr rhythm of donor SCN Ralph et al. 1990
Entrainment • Entrainment by light, temperature, or • arousing stimuli. • Photic entrainment in mammals due to • retinohypothalamic path to SCN. • Rods and cones not needed for entrainment! • Search for new receptors in ganglion cell • layer led to melanopsin. • Melanopsin ganglion cells directly activated • by light, indirectly by rods and cones. • Huge dendrites and receptive fields, • insensitive to light, but stable (no adaptation)
Projections of Melanopsin Neurons • Melanopsin neurons provide most of input to SCN. • Provide input to pretectal nucleus for pupillary reflex. • Provide input to intergeniculate leaflet of thalamus. IGLSCN. • IGL needed for arousing inputs to clock.
Entrainment by Arousal • Clock can be shifted by food, exercise, footshock and sex. • Allow animals to adjust rhythms to biologically significant opportunities. • Like light, shift can be up to 3 hours. • Shifts depend on phase—Light shifts best in dark phase, arousal shifts best in light phase.
Intergeniculate Leaflet: Arousal Shifts Circadian Rhythms Cain et al. 2001
Arousal Shifts Circadian Rhythms in Hamsters * Arousal (footshock, exercise, reward) Cain et al. 2001
Evolution of Retina? • How could eye evolve? Greatest problem for Cajal. • Circadian clock with direct access to light. • Light detectors, no spatial information—direct input to clock. • Eye cup—Spatial information, focussing, with pupil and lens later. • Dark and light vision (cones and rods) with adaptation. • Two eyeballs with muscles, for distance perception and fast movements in space.
Non-photic entrainment: IGL to SCN SCN Clock ?
Circadian Genes • How does endogenous clock work? • Clock mechanism found in plants, simple animals and many body cells. • Clock genes found in mutant fruit flies. How? • Take the flies who fly at odd hours. Map genes. • per: No rhythm, long rhythms, short rhythms. • Tim, cry, dbt. • Map genes onto 4 fly chromosomes. • Study functions of proteins: PER, TIM, DBT.
Mutations Alter Rhythms in Flys and Mice • per, tim are needed for 24 hr rhythms. • Mutations lead to short, long or no rhythm. • dbt mutations alter enzyme, casein kinase, leading to short rhythm in Drosophila. • Homologous genes (per1-3, cry, tau) found in mice and humans. • Transcription factors Clock and Cycle start each cycle. These are also regulated.
Clock Genes and Negative Feedback • per, cry genes transcribed in nucleus. • Per, Cry proteins are translated in cytoplasm. • Per/Cry dimers inhibit Clock/Cycle transcription factors in nucleus. • Less Per, Cry less inhibition. • New per, cry transcribed 24 hrs later. • Tau gene makes a casein kinase that degrades Per.
Molecular Model of Clock Nonphotic input from thalamus IGL? Gene transcription proteins Negative feedback loop Photic input from retina