1 / 59

HUMAN DEVELOPMENT 1 PSYCHOLOGY 3050: Thinking in Symbols: The Development of Representation (Ch 5)

This chapter explores the development of symbolic thinking and representation in human cognition. It discusses young children's interpretation of pictures and models, the appearance/reality distinction, and Jean Piaget's theories on cognitive development.

slower
Download Presentation

HUMAN DEVELOPMENT 1 PSYCHOLOGY 3050: Thinking in Symbols: The Development of Representation (Ch 5)

An Image/Link below is provided (as is) to download presentation Download Policy: Content on the Website is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use and may not be sold / licensed / shared on other websites without getting consent from its author. Content is provided to you AS IS for your information and personal use only. Download presentation by click this link. While downloading, if for some reason you are not able to download a presentation, the publisher may have deleted the file from their server. During download, if you can't get a presentation, the file might be deleted by the publisher.

E N D

Presentation Transcript


  1. HUMAN DEVELOPMENT 1PSYCHOLOGY 3050:Thinking in Symbols: The Development of Representation (Ch 5) Dr. Jamie Drover SN-3094, 864-8383 e-mail -- jrdrover@mun.ca Winter Semester, 2013

  2. Learning to Use Symbols • Symbols: external referents for objects and events. • Representational Insight: Knowledge that an entity can stand for something other than itself.

  3. Young Children’s Interpretation of Pictures and Models • DeLoache (1987) had 2- and 3-year-old children search for a toy hidden in a room. • Earlier, they are shown a model room that illustrates where the toy is. • They then have to find the toy in the room. • Then have to find the model toy in the model room.

  4. Young Children’s Interpretation of Pictures and Models 3-year-olds possess representational insight. 2.5-year-olds do not

  5. Young Children’s Interpretation of Pictures and Models • If a picture is used instead of a scale model, 2.5-year-olds show representational insight, whereas 2-year-olds do not (DeLoache 1987). • These findings may reflect difficulty with dual-representation. • A model is its own item, worthy of its own attention. • When models are made less interesting, performance changes.

  6. Young Children’s Interpretation of Pictures and Models • When models were viewed through a window, 2.5 year-olds’ performance was better than on the model task. • When 3-year-olds were allowed to play with the model beforehand, performance decreased. • DeLoache et al. (1997) designed a task that did not require dual representation.

  7. Young Children’s Interpretation of Pictures and Models • “credible shrinking room studies” -- 2.5 yr olds can succeed • “shrinking machine” can shrink room • shown “Terry the Troll” • machine “shrinks” (then enlarges) Terry

  8. Young Children’s Interpretation of Pictures and Models

  9. Young Children’s Interpretation of Pictures and Models • Standard model task – hide Terry in large room • Room was “shrunk” • 2.5 yr can find Terry in small room • No need for representational link between model and the room, instead -- large and small room believed to be the same thing • no dual representation needed

  10. Young Children’s Interpretation of Pictures and Models • Even an 18 month-old will show basic symbolic play. • But this is not necessarily dual representation. • DeLoache et al. (1998) presented pictures to 9 to 19 month-old children from the US and the Ivory Coast. • The youngest children treated them as objects.

  11. Young Children’s Interpretation of Pictures and Models • By 19 months of age, they realized the picture represented something else.

  12. The Appearance/Reality Distinction • The knowledge that the appearance of an object does not necessarily correspond to its reality.

  13. The Appearance/Reality Distinction • De Vries (1969) studied qualitative identity • Children were familiarized with a trained cat. • The cat was then fitted with a dog mask. • 3-year-olds believed the mask changed the identity of the cat. • Flavell (1986) poured white milk into a red glass while young children were watching. • Showed children a sponge that looked like a rock.

  14. The Appearance/Reality Distinction • They were asked what does it look like to your eyes right now? • Asked, what is it, really and truly? • Made two kinds of errors. • Phenomenismerrors: said milk was really and truly red. • Intellectual realism: Said the fake rock looked like a sponge.

  15. The Appearance/Reality Distinction Young children’s poor performance on appearance/reality distinction tasks is surprisingly pervasive. Might stem from problems with dual encoding. They have trouble representing an object in more than one form at a time.

  16. Jean Piaget • A Swiss philosopher/psychologist first trained as a biologist. • Has had the greatest impact on developmental psychology. • Emphasized the role of children in development. • Children are not incomplete adults. • Think differently, qualitative differences.

  17. Assumptions of Piaget’s Theory • We develop in discrete stages. • Cognitive development is through a series of transformations. • But underlying functions are continuous. • Mechanisms of cognitive development are domain-general (homogeneity of function).

  18. Assumptions of Piaget’s Theory • Children are not passive creatures, they are intrinsically active and possess an innate curiosity and seek stimulation. • The motivation for development is within the child. • They are primarily responsible for their own development.

  19. Assumptions of Piaget’s Theory • Cognition is a constructive process. • We interpret the world through our own personal perspective, ie, through what we already know. • Constructivism • Children at different levels construct different realities.

  20. The Constructive Nature of Cognition • They come to know objects by acting on them – action schemes. • Scheme: the basic unit of knowledge. • These action schemes become internalized – operations or operational schemes.

  21. Functional Invariants • Processes that characterize all biological systems (including intelligence) and operate throughout the lifespan. • Organization: Through organization, every intellectual operation is related to all other acts of intelligence. • Structures/schemes are not independent, but are coordinated. • Domain general

  22. Functional Invariants • Adaptation: the organism’s ability to adjust its structures to environmental demands. • Assimilation: the incorporation of new information in already existing schemes. • Accommodation: a current scheme is changed to incorporate new information.

  23. Assimilation and Accommodation • Knowledge is constructed by these processes. • Every act of intelligence involves both. • One may predominate over the other. • Play, imitation

  24. Equilibration • The organism’s attempt to keep its cognitive structures in balance. • When information does not match current schemes, disequilibrium results. • Achieved through alteration of cognitive structures (e.g., accommodation). • The child may also assimilate.

  25. Stages of Development • The order of the stages are invariant and culturally universal. • Development is epigenetic • Based on bidirectional interactions between structure and function. • Later development is based on earlier development. • New structure is a transformation of an earlier one.

  26. The Sensorimotor Stage • Birth to 2 years. • Intelligence is limited to one’s own actions on the environment. • Do not form mental representations. • Understand only what is physically present. • Knowledge progresses from sensorimotor to representational thinking.

  27. The Sensorimotor Stage • There is a change in personal perspective. • Learn to differentiate themselves from the external world. • There are six substages • 1) the use of reflexes: Birth to 1 month • Use reflexes to interpret the world • They apply reflexes to objects and assimilate them to their schemes.

  28. The Sensorimotor Stage • Highly restricted in what they can know. • They do not behave intentionally, but can adapt. • 2) Primary circular reactions: 1 to 4 months • Reflexes are extended, new patterns of behavior are acquired. • Can modify reflex schemes.

  29. The Sensorimotor Stage • Primary Circular Reactions: the first class of acquired repetitive behaviors. • Based on hereditary reflexes • Show primitive signs of intentionality.

  30. The Sensorimotor Stage • 3) Secondary Circular Reactions: 4 to 8 months. • Not based on reflexes, but represent the first acquired new behaviors. • These behaviors first appear by chance. • 4) Coordination of secondary circular reactions: 8 to 12 months. • Show goal-directed behavior and cause and effect.

  31. The Sensorimotor Stage • Coordinates secondary circular reactions. • 5) Tertiary Circular Reactions: 12 to 18 months. • Characterized by clear means/end differentiation. • Can alter existing schemes directly related to obtaining a solution. • Show increasing locomotive abilities. • Show a peak in curiosity.

  32. The Sensorimotor Stage • Still cannot form mental representations. • Solve problems through trial and error. • 6) Invention of new means through mental combinations. • Symbolic functioning is first seen. • New means are invented through mental combinations.

  33. The Sensorimotor Stage • Show symbolic function through language, deferred imitation, gestures, and mental imagery.

  34. The Development of Operations • In the three stages following the sensorimotor stage, children can form mental representations. • Preoperations: 2-7 • Concrete Operations: 7-11 • Formal Operations: Begins at 11

  35. The Development of Operations • Operations: Cognitive schemes that describe ways in which children act on their world. • Mental; require the use of symbols • Derive from action. They are internalized actions. • Exist within an organized system. • All cognitive operations are integrated.

  36. The Development of Operations • Operations are logical and follow rules. • Reversibility – knowledge that an operation can be reversed. Two types: • negation – an operation can be negated, or inverted • (5+2 = 7; 7-2 = 5) • compensation -- change in one dimension offset by changes in another -- a tall thin man and a short fat man can weigh the same

  37. The Transition from Preoperational to Concrete Operational Thought • Thinking in the preoperations stage is intuitive, lacking logic. • More concerned with appearance than logic • Conservation • The realization that an entity stays the same despite changes in its form. • This is the sign that one has achieved concrete operations.

  38. The Transition from Preoperational to Concrete Operational Thought • E.g. conservation of liquid (volume). • 5-year-olds cannot solve this problem. 8-year-olds can solve the problem and explain why.

  39. The Transition from Preoperational to Concrete Operational Thought • The pre-operational child thinks intuitively. • If the liquid is poured back into the original container, preoperational children claim the amounts are equal. • This does not produce contradiction (disequilibrium) in the preoperational child. • But it does in older children. They will soon accommodate.

  40. The Transition from Preoperational to Concrete Operational Thought • Conservation does not develop simultaneously for all properties of materials. • Number before mass before weight before volume • Note that there is heterogeneity here. Conservation of Number

  41. The Transition from Preoperational to Concrete Operational Thought • Reversibility • Preoperational children can not apply negation or compensation to conservation problems. • Centration v. Decentration • Preoperational children’s perception is centered. • They make judgments based on the most salient aspect

  42. The Transition from Preoperational to Concrete Operational Thought • Concrete operational children are decentered. • Can remove their attention from specific aspects of the conservation problem and make decision based on all dimensions. • Centration is not limited to conservation tasks but is found in everyday life • Use height to estimate age

  43. The Transition from Preoperational to Concrete Operational Thought • Egocentricity • Preoperational children assume that others see the world as they do. • This permeates their complete cognitive world. • Perhaps this egocentricity is adaptive.

  44. Transition from Concrete to Formal Operational Thought • In early adolescence, children’s thoughts are no longer applied to the concrete. • Not limited to tangible facts or object • Hypothetico-Deductive Reasoning • The benchmark of formal operations. • They can generate hypotheses. • Can think solely on the basis of symbols.

  45. Transition from Concrete to Formal Operational Thought • Can generate ideas not yet experienced. • Thinking like a scientist • Can think inductively. • Go from specific observations to broad generalizations. • Hypotheses are generated then systematically tested.

  46. Transition from Concrete to Formal Operational Thought • Pendulum problem • Given four factors that can affect pendulum speed • String length, weight of object, height of release, force of push. • Must formulate a hypothesis • Vary a single factor while holding the others constant.

  47. Transition from Concrete to Formal Operational Thought • Preoperational children can carry out the first step. • Concrete operational children can’t get the right answer. • Can’t isolate a variable. • Thinking About Thinking • Can examine the content of their own thought.

  48. Transition from Concrete to Formal Operational Thought • Can acquire new information from internal reflection. • Reflective abstraction: a rearrangement, by means of thought, of some matter previously presented to the subject in a rough or immediate form. • Egocentricity • Adolescents demonstrate centration.

  49. Transition from Concrete to Formal Operational Thought • Believe that their abstract ideas are unique to them. • Adolescents are extremely self-conscious. • Playing to an imaginary audience. • Leads to the personal fable • Belief in uniqueness and invulnerability. • May explain reckless behavior • May be adaptive by ensuring experimentation and independence.

  50. Transition from Concrete to Formal Operational Thought • It’s debatable whether adolescents or even adults are the logical thinkers Piaget thought they were. • Formal operational thought is used by adults in some contexts, but not in other.

More Related